Main Article Content
Abstract
A general economic principle is that when evaluating the costs of a decision, sunk costs should not be considered, and the decision-maker should consider only those costs incurred as a result of making that decision. However, anecdotal and empirical evidence has shown that people are influenced by sunk costs when making decisions, thereby committing the sunk-cost fallacy. A corpus of research has established that this fallacy occurs among nations and cultures to differing extents or degrees. None of the earlier studies, however, focused on Nigerians. This study, therefore, investigates whether Nigerians, too, commit this fallacy and then identifies factors that affect Nigerians' susceptibility to the fallacy. Employing a binary logit model showed that about 49 per cent of the respondents to questions based on a decision-making vignette committed the sunk-cost fallacy. The results also showed that locus of cost responsibility (whether the decision maker or another person bore the cost on behalf of the decision maker) and ethnicity (whether the decision maker is Yoruba or not) were significant determinants of susceptibility to sunk-cost fallacy. This suggests that in Nigeria, the sunk-cost fallacy is intrapersonal and more prevalent among Yorubas than among Hausas or Igbos. Therefore, the sunk-cost fallacy is ubiquitous and more likely in personal decisions than decisions made on behalf of others.
References
- Akinbobola, O. I., & Ehigie, B. O. (2012). Influence of decision responsibility and consequence on escalation of commitment in corporate investment. International Journal of Applied Psychology, 2(5), 119-125. https://doi.org/10.5923/j.ijap.20120205.07
- Arkes, H. R., & Ayton, P. (1999). The sunk cost and Concorde effects: Are humans less rational than lower animals? Psychological Bulletin, 125(5), 591-600. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.591
- Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(85)90049-4
- Aronson, E. 1968. Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In R. Abelson, E. Aronson, W. McGuire, T. Newcomb, M. Rosenberg & P. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook, 5–27. Chicago: Rand McNally.
- Astebro, T., Jeffrey, S. A., & Adomdza, G. K. (2007). Inventor perseverance after being told to quit: The role of cognitive biases. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20(3), 253–272. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.554
- Baron, J., Granato, L., Spranca, M., & Teubal, E. (1993). Decision-making biases in children and early adolescents: Exploratory studies. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 39, 22–46.
- Bazerman, M. H., Beekun, R. I., & Schoorman, F. D. (1982). Performance evaluation in a dynamic context: A laboratory study of the impact of a prior commitment to the ratee. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67(6), 873–876. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.67.6.873
- Besanko, D., & Braeutigan, R. R. (2011). Microeconomics (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley.
- Bernard, H. R. (2006). Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches (4th Ed.). Oxford: AltaMira Press.
- Biala, M. I. (2022, in press). Do decision-making professionals commit the sunk-cost fallacy? Malete Journal of Accounting and Finance, 3(1).
- Bornstein, B. H., & Chapman, G. B. (1995). Learning lessons from sunk costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1(4), 251-269. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.1.4.251
- Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of action: Toward theoretical progress. The Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 39-61. https://doi.org/10.2307/258647
- Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125 (3), 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367
- Chow, C. W., Harrison, P., Lindquist, T., & Wu, A. (1997). Escalating commitment to unprofitable projects: Replication and cross-cultural extension. Management Accounting Research, 8(3), 347-361. https://doi.org/10.1006/mare.1996.0046
- Conlon, D. E., & Garland, H. (1993). The role of project completion information in resource allocation decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36(2), 402–413. https://doi.org/10.5465/256529
- Davis, M., & Bobko, P. (1986). Contextual effects on escalation processes in public sector decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 37(1), 121-138. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(86)90048-8
- Dawkins, R., & T. R. Carlisle. (1976). Parental investment, mate desertion and a fallacy. Nature, 262(5564), 131–33. https://doi.org/10.1038/262131a0
- Falchetta, G. (2015). The sunk cost fallacy: A literature review and an empirical test. (Unpublished bachelor thesis). School of Economics and Management, Free University of Bolzano.
- Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9781503620766
- Forrest, V., & Hasseldine, J. (2016). Escalating commitment: business investments and CSR. Honors Theses and Capstones. 281. https://scholars.unh.edu/honors/281
- Garland, H. (1990). Throwing good money after bad: The effect of sunk costs on the decision to escalate commitment to an ongoing project. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(6), 728-731. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.75.6.728
- Garland, H., & Newport, S. (1991) Effects of absolute and relative sunk costs on the decision to persist with a course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 48(1), 55-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90005-E
- Geiger, S. W., Robertson, C. J., & Irwin, J. G. (1998). The impact of cultural values on escalation of commitment. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 6(2), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1108/eb028883
- Heng, C. S., Tan, B. C. Y., & Wei, K. K. (2003). De-escalation of commitment in software projects: Who matters? What matters? Information & Management, 41(1), 99–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-7206(03)00030-2
- Jeffrey, C. (1992). The relation of judgment, personal involvement, and experience in the audit of bank loans. Accounting Review, 67, 802–819.
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
- Keil, M., Tan, B. C. Y., Wei, K.-K., Saarinen, T., Tuunainen. V., & Wassenaar, A. (2000). A cross-cultural study on escalation of commitment behavior in software projects. MIS Quarterly, 24(2), 299-325. https://doi.org/10.2307/3250940
- Klaczynski, P. (2001). Framing effects on adolescent task representations, analytic and heuristic processing, and decision making: Implications for the normative/descriptive gap. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22(3), 289–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(01)00085-5
- Larrick, R. P., Nisbett, R. E., & Morgan, J. N. (1993). Who uses the cost-benefit rules of choice? Implications for the normative status of microeconomic theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 56(3), 331-347. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1993.1058
- Maréchal, J.-C. (2010). Editor's message: the sunk cost fallacy of deep drilling. Hydrogeology Journal 18, 287–289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10040-009-0515-2
- Nash, J. S. (2017). The Sunk cost effect: Short-term behavioural evidence in adults. (Unpublished doctoral thesis). The University of Queensland.
- Navarro, A. D. & Fantino, E. (2008). The sunk-time effect: An exploration. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22(3), 252-270. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.624
- Olivola, C. Y. (2018) The interpersonal sunk-cost effect. Psychological Science, 29(7),1072-1083. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797617752641
- Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and persons. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
- Pattison, K. F., Zentall, T. R., & Watanabe, S. (2012). Sunk cost: pigeons (Columba livia), too, show bias to complete a task rather than shift to another. Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C.: 1983), 126(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023826
- Peterson, R. A. (2001). On the use of college students in social science research: insights from a second-order meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(3), 450–461. https://doi.org/10.1086/323732
- Rego, S., Arantes, J., & Magalhães, P. (2016). Is there a sunk cost effect in committed relationships? Current Psychology, 37, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9529-9
- Roth, S., Thomas, R. & Straus, L. (2015). On the sunk-cost effect in economic decision-making: A meta-analytic review. Business Research, 8, 99-138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40685-014-0014-8
- Soman, D. (2001). The mental accounting of sunk time costs: Why time is not like money. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 14(3), 169-185. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.370
- Schreiber-Gregory, D, & Bader, K. (2018). Logistic and linear regression assumptions: Violation recognition and control. Southeast SAS User Group (SESUG). Conference Paper 247, https://www.researchgate.net/publication/341354759.
- Schmidt, J. B., & Calantone, R. J. (2002). Escalation of commitment during new product development. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30, 103–118. https://doi.org/10.1177/03079459994362
- Schulz, A. K. D., & Cheng, M. M. (2002). Persistence in capital budgeting reinvestment decisions – personal responsibility antecedent and information asymmetry moderator: A note. Accounting and Finance, 42(1), 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-629X.00004
- Sleesman, D. J., Conlon, D. E., McNamara, G., & Miles, J. E. (2012). Cleaning up the big muddy: A meta-analytic review of the determinants of escalation of commitment. Academy of Management Journal, 55(3), 541–562. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2010.0696
- Sleesman, D. J., Lennard, A. C., McNamara, G., & Colon, D. E. (2018). Putting escalation of commitment in context: A multilevel review and analysis. Academy of Management Annals, 12(1), 178–207. https://doi.org/10.5465/annals.2016.0046.
- Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: A study of escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(1), 27-44. https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(76)90005-2
- Staw, B. M. (2016). Stumbling toward a social psychology of organizations: An autobiographical look at the direction of organizational research. The Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 3, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-041015-062524
- Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1989). Understanding behavior in escalation situations. Science, 246(4927), 216-220. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.246.4927.216
- Strough, J., de Bruin, W. B., Parker, A. M., Karns, T., Lemaster, P., Pichayayothin, N., & Stoiko, R. (2016). What were they thinking? Reducing sunk-cost bias in a life-span sample. Psychology and Aging, 31(7), 724-736. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000130
- Strough, J., Mehta, C. M., Mcfall, J. P., & Schuller, K. L. (2008). Are older adults less subject to the sunk-cost fallacy than younger adults? Psychological Science, 19(7), 650-652. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02138.x
- Sweis, B. M., Abraham, S. V., Schmidt, B. J., Seeland, K. D., MacDonald, A. W., Thomas, M. J., & Redish, A. D. (2018). Sensitivity to “sunk costs” in mice, rats, and humans. Science, 361(6398), 178–181. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aar8644
- Thaler, R. (1980). Toward s positive theory of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Behaviour and Organization, 1(1), 39-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(80)90051-7
- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683
- Webley, P., & Plaisier, Z. (1998). Mental accounting in childhood. Citizenship, Social and Economic Education, 3(2), 55-64. https://doi.org/10.2304/csee.1998.3.2.55
- Whyte, G. (1993). Escalating commitment in individual and group decision making: A prospect theory approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54(3), 430–455. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1993.1018
- Whyte, G. (1986). Escalating commitment to a course of action: A reinterpretation. The Academy of Management Review, 11(2), 311-321. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1986.4283111
- Wong, K. (2005). The role of risk in making decisions under escalation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 54(4), 584-607. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00236.x
- Wooldridge, J. M. (2006). Introductory econometrics: A modern approach (3rd ed.) Mason: Thomson South-Western
- Yoder, C. Y., Mancha, R. & Agrawal, N. (2014). Culture-related factors affect sunk cost bias. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19(4), 105–118. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0101086
- Zhang, L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Your money or your self-esteem: Threatened egotism promotes costly entrapment in losing endeavors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(7), 881–893. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206287120
- Zeng, J., Zhang, Q., Chen, C., Yu, R., & Gong, Q. (2013). An fMRI study on sunk cost effect. Brain Research, 1519, 63–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.001
Article Details
Copyright (c) 2022 Musa Ilias Biala
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
Management & Economics Research Journal is licensed under a (CC BY-NC 4.0).
The following guidelines apply to all users:
- 1-Individuals may view, download, print, or save Journal content for research and teaching purposes.
- 2-Any use and/or copies of this journal in whole or part must include the customary bibliographic citation, including author attribution, date, article title, the Journal name, and website address.
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a) Authors retain copyright and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a (CC BY-NC 4.0) that allows others to share and adapt the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b) Authors can enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c) Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) before and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges and earlier and greater citation of published work. Where authors include such work in an institutional repository or on their website, we request that they include a statement acknowledging the Management & Economics Research journal, including the journal's name, the volume and issue, and a web link to the journal item.
PhD
Other articles by the same author(s)
- Musa Ilias Biala, Yusuf Toyin Yusuf, Information and Communications Technology and the Level of Income in Africa , Management & Economics Research Journal: Vol. 4 No. 1 (2022): March